Insights into Spanish metrical structure through language games
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.7557/1.13.2.7701Keywords:
Spanish metrical structure, quantity sensitivity, trochee, language gamesAbstract
Some Spanish language games involve reordering the syllables of the words. However, the stressed syllable of the game word does not always match the stressed syllable in the original Spanish word and/or the position of the stress in the original Spanish word. Using game words found in different sources, a corpus of 261 words from different Spanish games was created to account for the games’ stress patterns. The metrical structure of the language games (e.g., Vesre, which is a game in Argentina and Uruguay) was analyzed. The results of the analysis suggest that the game words’ metrical structures are composed of quantity-sensitive, right-aligned trochee, in which the stress falls in penultimate position in vowel-final words, and consonant-final words generally have final stress, confirming previous proposals about the default metrical structure of Spanish (Harris, 1983; Núñez-Cedeño and Morales-Front, 1999). Overall, these Spanish language games illustrate The Emergence of The Unmarked (TETU) in metrical structure, providing evidence of Spanish stress assignment, whereby the unmarked stress pattern is a right-aligned quantity-sensitive trochee.
References
BAlvord, S. M. (2003). The psychological unreality of quantity sensitivity in Spanish: experimental evidence. Southwest Journal of Linguistics, 22(2), 1-13.
Becker, Michael & Potts Kathryn Flack. (2011). “The emergence of the unmarked”. In Marc van Oostendorp, Colin J. Ewen, Elizabeth Hume, and Keren Rice (eds.), The Blackwell Companion to Phonology, pp. 1363–1379. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444335262.wbctp0058
Bohrn, A. (2010). Sobre la morfología rioplatense: el vesre. [Conference Presentation] Romania Nova Conference. Universidade Estadual de Campinas, São Paulo, Brasil
Bohrn, A. (2015). “Inversión silábica y realización de género y número: el caso del" vesre rioplatense”. RASAL lingüística, (1), 29-48.
Bohrn, A. (2018). “Vesre, paronomasia y acortamiento: ¿Qué puede decirnos el lunfardo sobre el diseño de la gramática?” Punto. Revista de vanguardia literaria, (2)
Bohrn, A. (2021). El vesre en el origen del lunfardo. Gramma, 32(67).
Conde, O. (2014). “Las formas anagramáticas en lunfardo”. Gramma, 24(51), 87-94.
Harris, J. W. (1983). Syllable structure and stress in Spanish. A nonlinear analysis. Linguistic Inquiry Monographs Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Hualde, J. I. (2014). Los sonidos del español: Spanish Language edition. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511719943
Kager, R. (1999). Optimality theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511812408
McCarthy, John J., & Prince, Alan. 1994. “The emergence of the unmarked: Optimality in prosodic morphology”. Papers from the Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society 24: 333–379
McCarthy, John. 2008. Doing optimality theory: Applying theory to data. Malden: Blackwell Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444301182
Núnez Cedeno, R.A. Morales-Front,Alfonso (1999): Fonología generativa contemporánea de la lengua española. Washington (DC): Georgetown University Press.
Piñeros, Carlos-Eduardo. 1998. Prosodic Morphology in Spanish: constraint interaction in word. formation. Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, Columbus. Rutgers Optimality Archive.
Piñeros, Carlos-Eduardo. 2016. The phonological weight of Spanish syllables. In The syllable and stress: Studies in honor of James W. Harris, ed. Rafael Núñez Cedeño, 271–314. Berlin: de Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9781614515975-011
Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (2004). Optimality Theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Optimality Theory in phonology: A reader, 1-71. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470759400
Roca, I. (1990). Diachrony and synchrony in Spanish word stress. Journal of Linguistics 26.133-164. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700014456
Roca, Iggy. 2006. The Spanish stress window. In Optimality-Theoretic studies in Spanish phonology, Vol. Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today 99, eds. Fernando Martínez-Gil and Sonia Colina, 239–277. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/la.99.10roc
Sorbet, P. (2014). Análisis lingüístico del vesre porteño. Roczniki Humanistyczne, 62(05), 123-134.
Sorbet, P. (2016a). Aproximación a la descripción lingüística del vesre porteño. In Relecturas y nuevos horizontes en los estudios hispánicos. Vol. 4: Lingüística y didáctica de la lengua española (pp. 173-183). Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Śląskiego.
Sorbet, P. (2016b). En torno al tratamiento lexicográfico de los vesreísmos. Itinerarios, (23), 141-153.
Sorbet, P. (2017). El vesre: un mecanismo de creación léxica. Studia iberystyczne, (16), 151-167. https://doi.org/10.12797/SI.16.2017.16.09
Sorbet, P. (2019). Los vesreísmos peruanos. Kwartalnik Neofilologiczny, (4).
Sorbet, P. (2020). Los vesreísmos como bases léxicas. In Nowikow W., López González AM, Pawlikowska M., Baran M., Sobczak W.(red.), Lingüística hispánica teórica y aplicada. Estudios léxico-gramaticales didácticos y traductológicos, Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego, Łódź-Kraków 2020; Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego. https://doi.org/10.18778/8220-201-4.29
Torres-Tamarit, Francesc & Martínez-Paricio, Violeta (2024). The prosody of Spanish acronyms. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 42: 815–848. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-023-09599-w
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2024 Felix Fonseca Quesada
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.