Måleegenskaper ved de norske versjonene av Ravens matriser (Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM)/Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM))
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.21337/0055Emneord (Nøkkelord):
Kognitive evner, Barn i skolealderSammendrag
Beskrivelse: Ravens matriser består av flere varianter der Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM) (5– 11 år) og Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM) (8–65 år) kan anvendes for barn og unge. Testen ble utviklet av John C. Raven, og SPM ble først publisert i 1938, mens CPM-versjonen ble publisert i 1998. Testen er ment å måle non-verbal intelligens eller evne til abstrakt resonnering ved at barnet skal finne systemet bak hvordan ulike geometriske figurer er satt opp. Begge versjonene av Raven har oppgaver med økende vanskelighetsgrad organisert i ulike sett. Testen skåres ved at antallet riktige svar summeres opp til en totalskår. Dette kan konverteres til prosentiler basert på normtabellene i manualene. Testen administreres vanligvis uten tidsbegrensning for barn og unge, og det stilles krav om at de som skal anvende testen er sertifisert for å anvende evnetester eller er psykologer. Pearson Assessment har rettighetene til salg og distribusjon av testen i Skandinavia og internasjonalt (pearsonassessment.no).
Litteratursøk: Til sammen ble 15 norske og 24 svenske/danske artikler inkludert i oppsummeringen. Ingen av disse var rene psykometriske studier, men stort sett studier der Raven ble brukt for å måle intelligens enten som utkommevariabel, kontrollvariabel eller for å beskrive gruppen. Litt under halvparten av studiene var basert på kliniske grupper (for eksempel autisme, epilepsi eller døve), men de øvrige studiene var basert på skolebarn fra 5–16 (SPM) og 5–7 (CPM) år.
Psykometri: Ingen av studiene hadde gjennomført adekvate studier av testens reliabilitet og det er heller ikke gjennomført normstudier basert på norske eller svensk/danske utvalg. Resultatene fra studiene støtter begrepsvaliditeten til testen som et godt mål på abstrakt resonneringsevne ved generelt høye korrelasjoner med andre kognitive tester.
Konklusjon: Det er god dokumentasjon på testens begrepsvaliditet, men det mangler studier av testens reliabilitet basert på norske eller svenske/danske utvalg. Det er heller ikke normstudier fra Skandinavia, noe som er problematisk ved klinisk bruk av testen.
Referanser
Andersson, U. (2007). The contribution of working memory to children's mathematical word problem solving. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21(9), 1201-1216. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1317
Andersson, U. (2008). Working memory as a predictor of written arithmetical skills in children: The importance of central executive functions. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78(2), 181-203. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709907x209854
Bosnes, O. (2005). Comparison of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale/Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised, and Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence in a Norwegian clinical sample. Tidsskrift for Norsk Psykologforening, 42(7), 598-602. https://psykologtidsskriftet.no/fagartikkel/2005/07/en-sammenligning-av-wechsler-adult-intelligence-scalewechsler-intelligence-scale
Brynskov, C., Eigsti, I.-M., Jorgensen, M., Lemcke, S., Bohn, O.-S. & Krojgaard, P. (2017). Syntax and morphology in Danish-speaking children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(2), 373-383. https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2962-7
Bylund, B., Cervin, T., Finnström, O., Gäddlin, P.-O., Leijon, I., Mård, S. et al. (2000). Very low birth weight children at 9 years : School performance and behaviour in relation to risk factors. Prenatal and Neonatal Medicine, 5(2), 124-133.
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A. & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: a theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological review, 97(3), 404-431. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.404
Cattell, R. (1940). A culture-free intelligence test I. Journal of Educational Psychology, 31(3), 161-179. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0059043
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G. & Aiken, L. (2003). Applied Multiple Regression / Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (3rd utg.). London: Lawrence Erlbaum associates.
Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika, 16, 297–334. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02310555
Cronbach, L. J. & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological bulletin, 52(4), 281-302. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0040957
Dahlin, K. I. (2011). Effects of working memory training on reading in children with special needs. Reading and Writing, 24(4), 479-491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9238-y
Dumontheil, I. & Klingberg, T. (2012). Brain activity during a visuospatial working memory task predicts arithmetical performance 2 years later. Cerebral Cortex, 22(5), 1078-1085. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr175
Elbro, C. & Buch-Iversen, I. (2013). Activation of background knowledge for inference making: Effects on reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(6), 435-452. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.774005
European Federation of Pscyhologists’ Association - EFPA. (2013). EFPA Review model for the description and evaluation of psychological tests: Test review form and notes for reviewers, v 4.2.6: European Federation of Pscyhologists’ Association (EFPA).
Fasting, R. B. & Lyster, S.-A. H. (2005). The effects of computer technology in assisting the development of literacy in young struggling readers and spellers. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 20(1), 21-40. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1080/0885625042000319061
Fischbein, S., Guttman, R. & Nathan, M. (1999). Genetic and environmental influences on pupil performances. Twin Research, 2(3), 183-195. https://doi.org/10.1375/136905299320565852
Flynn, J. R. (2011). Secular changes in intelligence. I R. J. Sternberg & S. B. Kaufman (Red.), The Cambridge handbook of intelligence (s. 647-663). New York: Cambridge University press.
Forssman, L., Eninger, L., Tillman, C. M., Rodriguez, A. & Bohlin, G. (2012). Cognitive functioning and family risk factors in relation to symptom behaviors of ADHD and ODD in adolescents. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16(4), 284-294. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054710385065
Frostad, P. (1995). Ravens progressive matriser brukt på et utvalg hørselshemmede elever : notat. Trondheim: P. Frostad.
Gellert, A. S. & Elbro, C. (2013). Do experimental measures of word learning predict vocabulary development over time? A study of children from grade 3 to 4. Learning and Individual Differences, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.04.006
Green, K., Tønnessen, F., Tambs, K., Thoresen, M. & Bjertness, E. (2009). Dyslexia: Group screening among 15-16-year-olds in Oslo, Norway. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 53(3), 217-227. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313830902917246
Gustafson, S., Samuelsson, C., Johansson, E. & Wallmann, J. (2013). How simple is the simple view of reading? Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 57(3), 292-308. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313831.2012.656279
Gustafsson, J.-E. (1984). A unifying model for the structure of intellectual abilities. Intelligence, 8(3), 179-203. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(84)90008-4
Gustafsson, J.-E. & Wolff, U. (2015). Measuring fluid intelligence at age four. Intelligence, 50, 175-185. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.04.008
Høie, B., Mykletun, A., Sommerfelt, K., Bjornæs, H., Skeidsvoll, H. & Waaler, P. E. (2005). Seizure-related factors and non-verbal intelligence in children with epilepsy a population-based study from Western Norway. Seizure, 14(4), 223-231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.seizure.2004.10.006
Høie, B., Mykletun, A., Waaler, P., Skeidsvoll, H. & Sommerfelt, K. (2006). Executive functions and seizure-related factors in children with epilepsy in western Norway. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 48(6), 519-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0012162206001095
Høie, B., Sommerfelt, K., Waaler, P., Alsaker, F., Skeidsvoll, H. & Mykletun, A. (2006). Psychosocial problems and seizure-related factors in children with epilepsy. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 48(3), 213-219. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0012162206000454
Høie, B., Sommerfelt, K., Waaler, P. E., Alsaker, F. D., Skeidsvoll, H. & Mykletun, A. (2008). The combined burden of cognitive, executive function, and psychosocial problems in children with epilepsy: A population-based study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 50(7), 530-536. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03015.x
Kvist, A. V. & Gustafsson, J.-E. (2008). The relation between fluid intelligence and the general factor as a function of cultural background: A test of Cattell's Investment theory. Intelligence, 36(5), 422-436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.08.004
Landmark, M. & Grinde, T. (1962). A group of normal children tested with five intelligence tests. Nordisk Psykologi, 14(4), 171-185. https://doi.org/10.1080/00291463.1962.10780151
Lervåg, A., Bråten, I. & Hulme, C. (2009). The cognitive and linguistic foundations of early reading development: A Norwegian latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 764-781. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014132
Lervåg, A. & Hulme, C. (2010). Predicting the growth of early spelling skills: Are there heterogeneous developmental trajectories? Scientific Studies of Reading, 14(6), 485-513. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888431003623488
Lord, F. M., Novic, M. R. & Birnbaum, A. (2008). Statistical theories of mental test scores. USA: Information age publishing inc.
Lyster, S.-A. H., Lervåg, A. O. & Hulme, C. (2016). Preschool morphological training produces long-term improvements in reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 29(6), 1269-1288. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9636-x
Marshalek, B., Lohman, D. & Snow, R. (1983). The complexity continuum in the radex and hierarchical models of intelligence. Intelligence, 7(2), 107-127. https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(83)90023-5
McGrew, K. S. (2005). The Cattell-Horn-Carroll Theory of Cognitive Abilities: Past, Present, and Future. I P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Red.), Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues. New York: Guilford press.
Nordberg, A., Dahlgren Sandberg, A. & Miniscalco, C. (2015). Story retelling and language ability in school-aged children with cerebral palsy and speech impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders / Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists, 50(6), 801-813. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12177
Nordberg, A., Miniscalco, C. & Lohmander, A. (2014). Consonant production and overall speech characteristics in school-aged children with cerebral palsy and speech impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16(4), 386-395. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2014.917440
Nutley, S. B., Darki, F. & Klingberg, T. (2014). Music practice is associated with development of working memory during childhood and adolescence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 926. https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00926
Oakland, T., Douglas, S. & Kane, H. (2016). The top ten standardized tests used with children and youth by school psychologists in 64 countries: A 24 year follow-up study. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 34, 166-176. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282915595303
Olsson, L., Ostergren, R. & Traff, U. (2016). Developmental dyscalculia: A deficit in the approximate number system or an access deficit? Cognitive Development, 39, 154-167. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.04.006
Pind, J., Gunnarsdóttir, E. K. & Jóhannesson, H. S. (2003). Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices: new school age norms and a study of the test's validity. Personality and Individual Differences, 34(3), 375-386. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00058-2
Primi, R. (2001). Complexity of geometric inductive reasoning tasks contribution to the understanding of fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30(1), 41-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00067-8
Raven, J. (1941). Standardization of progressive matrices, 1938. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 19, 137-150. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1941.tb00316.x
Raven, J. (2000a). Psychometrics, cognitive ability, and occupational performance. Review of Psychology, 7(1-2), 51-74.
Raven, J. (2000b). The Raven's Progressive Matrices: Change and Stability over Culture and Time. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 1-48. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0735
Raven, J. (2008a). Manual for the coloured progressive matrices and Crichton vocabulary scale (UK utg.). London: Pearson education.
Raven, J. (2008b). Manual for the standard progressive matrices - plus version and Mill Hill vocabulary scale (UK utg.). London: Pearson education.
Samuelsson, S., Finnstrom, O., Flodmark, O., Gaddlin, P.-O., Leijon, I. & Wadsby, M. (2006). A Longitudinal Study of Reading Skills Among Very-Low-Birthweight Children: Is There a Catch-up? Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31(9), 967-977. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsj108
Schmitt, N. (1996). Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha. Psychological assessment, 8(4), 350-353. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.350
Sijtsma, K. (2009). On the Use, the Misuse, and the Very Limited Usefulness of Cronbach's Alpha. Psychometrika, 74(1), 107-120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-008-9101-0
Skagerlund, K. & Traff, U. (2016a). Number processing and heterogeneity of developmental dyscalculia: Subtypes with different cognitive profiles and deficits. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(1), 36-50. https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219414522707
Skagerlund, K. & Traff, U. (2016b). Processing of space, time, and number contributes to mathematical abilities above and beyond domain-general cognitive abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 143, 85-101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.10.016
Solheim, O. J. (2011). The impact of reading self-efficacy and task value on reading comprehension scores in different item formats. Reading Psychology, 32(1), 1-27. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702710903256601
Svensson, I. & Jacobson, C. (2006). How persistent are phonological difficulties? A longitudinal study of reading retarded children. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 12(1), 3-20. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.296
Söderqvist, S., Nutley, S. B., Ottersen, J., Grill, K. M. & Klingberg, T. (2012). Computerized training of non-verbal reasoning and working memory in children with intellectual disability. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience Vol 6 Oct 2012, ArtID 271. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00271
Thirus, J., Starbrink, M. & Jansson, B. (2016). Relational frame theory, mathematical, and logical skills: A multiple exemplar training intervention to enhance intellectual performance. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 16(2), 141-155.
Träff, U. (2013). The contribution of general cognitive abilities and number abilities to different aspects of mathematics in children (Vol. 116:2, s. 139-156): Journal of experimental child psychology. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.007
Undheim, J. O. (1981). On intelligence I: Broad ability factors in 15-year-old children and Cattell's theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 22, 171–179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.1981.tb00391.x
Undheim, J. O. & Gustafsson, J. E. (1987). The Hierarchical Organization of Cognitive Abilities: Restoring General Intelligence Through the Use of Linear Structural Relations (LISREL). Multivariate Behavioral Research, Vol. 22 (2), 149-171. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2202_2
Vaskinn, A. & Egeland, J. (2012). Testbruksundersøkelsen: En oversikt over tester brukt av norske psykologer. Tidsskrift for norsk psykologforening, 49, 658-665. https://psykologtidsskriftet.no/fagartikkel/2012/07/testbruksundersokelsen-en-oversikt-over-tester-brukt-av-norske-psykologer
Vejleskov, H. (1968). An analysis of Raven matrix responses in fifth grade children: I. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 9(3), 177-186. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.1968.tb00533.x
Wolff, U. & Gustafsson, J.-E. (2015). Structure of phonological ability at age four. Intelligence, 53, 108-117. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.003
Wongupparaj, P., Kumari, V. & Morris, R. G. (2015). A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Raven's Progressive Matrices: Age groups and developing versus developed countries. Intelligence, 49, 1-9. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.008. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.008
Nedlastinger
Ytterligere filer
Publisert
Hvordan referere
Utgave
Seksjon
Lisens
Opphavsrett 2017 Fredrik Helland-Riise og Monica Martinussen
Dette verket er lisensiert under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.