The Psychometric Properties of the Norwegian version of Ravens Matrices (Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM)/Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM))

Authors

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.21337/0055

Keywords:

Kognitive evner, Barn i skolealder

Abstract

Description: There are different versions of Ravens Matrices including Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM) (5–11 years) and Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM) (8–65 years) which may be used for children and adolescents. The test was first developed by John C. Raven, and the SPM was published in 1938, and the CPM in 1998. The test is assumed to measure non-verbal intelligence or abstract reasoning ability with items consisting of geometrical figures where the task is to figure out the system underlying the figures. Both versions of Raven have items with increasing difficulty organized in different sets. The test is scored in terms of the number of correct answers, which is summed to a total score. The total score can be converted to percentiles based on norm tables included in the manuals. The test is usually administered without time constraints for children and adolescents, and the test user is required to be a psychologist or a certified user of ability tests.Pearson Assessment holds the copyright to Ravens Matrices, and is responsible for sale and distribution of the test in Scandinavia and internationally (pearsonassessment.no).
Literature search: A total of 15 Norwegian and 24 Swedish/Danish publications were included in the review. None of these were psychometric studies, but mainly studies where Raven had been used to measure intelligence either as an outcome variable, control variable or to describe the group. Approximately half of the included studies were based on clinical groups (e.g., autism, epilepsy or deaf), while the remaining studies included school children of 5–16 (SPM) and 5–7 (CPM) years.
Psychometrics: None of the included studies had performed adequate studies of test reliability and there were no norm studies based on Norwegian or Swedish/Danish samples. The findings support the construct validity of the test as a good measure of abstract reasoning ability by in general high correlations with other cognitive tests.
Conclusion: There is solid evidence supporting the construct validity of the test, but studies examining test reliability using Scandinavian samples are lacking. There are no norm studies from Scandinavia, which is problematic for clinical and applied test use.

References

Andersson, U. (2007). The contribution of working memory to children's mathematical word problem solving. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21(9), 1201-1216. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1317

Andersson, U. (2008). Working memory as a predictor of written arithmetical skills in children: The importance of central executive functions. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78(2), 181-203. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709907x209854

Bosnes, O. (2005). Comparison of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale/Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised, and Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence in a Norwegian clinical sample. Tidsskrift for Norsk Psykologforening, 42(7), 598-602. https://psykologtidsskriftet.no/fagartikkel/2005/07/en-sammenligning-av-wechsler-adult-intelligence-scalewechsler-intelligence-scale

Brynskov, C., Eigsti, I.-M., Jorgensen, M., Lemcke, S., Bohn, O.-S. & Krojgaard, P. (2017). Syntax and morphology in Danish-speaking children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(2), 373-383. https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2962-7

Bylund, B., Cervin, T., Finnström, O., Gäddlin, P.-O., Leijon, I., Mård, S. et al. (2000). Very low birth weight children at 9 years : School performance and behaviour in relation to risk factors. Prenatal and Neonatal Medicine, 5(2), 124-133.

Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A. & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: a theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological review, 97(3), 404-431. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.404

Cattell, R. (1940). A culture-free intelligence test I. Journal of Educational Psychology, 31(3), 161-179. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0059043

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G. & Aiken, L. (2003). Applied Multiple Regression / Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (3rd utg.). London: Lawrence Erlbaum associates.

Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika, 16, 297–334. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02310555

Cronbach, L. J. & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological bulletin, 52(4), 281-302. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0040957

Dahlin, K. I. (2011). Effects of working memory training on reading in children with special needs. Reading and Writing, 24(4), 479-491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9238-y

Dumontheil, I. & Klingberg, T. (2012). Brain activity during a visuospatial working memory task predicts arithmetical performance 2 years later. Cerebral Cortex, 22(5), 1078-1085. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr175

Elbro, C. & Buch-Iversen, I. (2013). Activation of background knowledge for inference making: Effects on reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(6), 435-452. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.774005

European Federation of Pscyhologists’ Association - EFPA. (2013). EFPA Review model for the description and evaluation of psychological tests: Test review form and notes for reviewers, v 4.2.6: European Federation of Pscyhologists’ Association (EFPA).

Fasting, R. B. & Lyster, S.-A. H. (2005). The effects of computer technology in assisting the development of literacy in young struggling readers and spellers. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 20(1), 21-40. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1080/0885625042000319061

Fischbein, S., Guttman, R. & Nathan, M. (1999). Genetic and environmental influences on pupil performances. Twin Research, 2(3), 183-195. https://doi.org/10.1375/136905299320565852

Flynn, J. R. (2011). Secular changes in intelligence. I R. J. Sternberg & S. B. Kaufman (Red.), The Cambridge handbook of intelligence (s. 647-663). New York: Cambridge University press.

Forssman, L., Eninger, L., Tillman, C. M., Rodriguez, A. & Bohlin, G. (2012). Cognitive functioning and family risk factors in relation to symptom behaviors of ADHD and ODD in adolescents. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16(4), 284-294. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054710385065

Frostad, P. (1995). Ravens progressive matriser brukt på et utvalg hørselshemmede elever : notat. Trondheim: P. Frostad.

Gellert, A. S. & Elbro, C. (2013). Do experimental measures of word learning predict vocabulary development over time? A study of children from grade 3 to 4. Learning and Individual Differences, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.04.006

Green, K., Tønnessen, F., Tambs, K., Thoresen, M. & Bjertness, E. (2009). Dyslexia: Group screening among 15-16-year-olds in Oslo, Norway. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 53(3), 217-227. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313830902917246

Gustafson, S., Samuelsson, C., Johansson, E. & Wallmann, J. (2013). How simple is the simple view of reading? Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 57(3), 292-308. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313831.2012.656279

Gustafsson, J.-E. (1984). A unifying model for the structure of intellectual abilities. Intelligence, 8(3), 179-203. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(84)90008-4

Gustafsson, J.-E. & Wolff, U. (2015). Measuring fluid intelligence at age four. Intelligence, 50, 175-185. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.04.008

Høie, B., Mykletun, A., Sommerfelt, K., Bjornæs, H., Skeidsvoll, H. & Waaler, P. E. (2005). Seizure-related factors and non-verbal intelligence in children with epilepsy a population-based study from Western Norway. Seizure, 14(4), 223-231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.seizure.2004.10.006

Høie, B., Mykletun, A., Waaler, P., Skeidsvoll, H. & Sommerfelt, K. (2006). Executive functions and seizure-related factors in children with epilepsy in western Norway. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 48(6), 519-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0012162206001095

Høie, B., Sommerfelt, K., Waaler, P., Alsaker, F., Skeidsvoll, H. & Mykletun, A. (2006). Psychosocial problems and seizure-related factors in children with epilepsy. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 48(3), 213-219. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0012162206000454

Høie, B., Sommerfelt, K., Waaler, P. E., Alsaker, F. D., Skeidsvoll, H. & Mykletun, A. (2008). The combined burden of cognitive, executive function, and psychosocial problems in children with epilepsy: A population-based study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 50(7), 530-536. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03015.x

Kvist, A. V. & Gustafsson, J.-E. (2008). The relation between fluid intelligence and the general factor as a function of cultural background: A test of Cattell's Investment theory. Intelligence, 36(5), 422-436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.08.004

Landmark, M. & Grinde, T. (1962). A group of normal children tested with five intelligence tests. Nordisk Psykologi, 14(4), 171-185. https://doi.org/10.1080/00291463.1962.10780151

Lervåg, A., Bråten, I. & Hulme, C. (2009). The cognitive and linguistic foundations of early reading development: A Norwegian latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 764-781. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014132

Lervåg, A. & Hulme, C. (2010). Predicting the growth of early spelling skills: Are there heterogeneous developmental trajectories? Scientific Studies of Reading, 14(6), 485-513. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888431003623488

Lord, F. M., Novic, M. R. & Birnbaum, A. (2008). Statistical theories of mental test scores. USA: Information age publishing inc.

Lyster, S.-A. H., Lervåg, A. O. & Hulme, C. (2016). Preschool morphological training produces long-term improvements in reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 29(6), 1269-1288. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9636-x

Marshalek, B., Lohman, D. & Snow, R. (1983). The complexity continuum in the radex and hierarchical models of intelligence. Intelligence, 7(2), 107-127. https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(83)90023-5

McGrew, K. S. (2005). The Cattell-Horn-Carroll Theory of Cognitive Abilities: Past, Present, and Future. I P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Red.), Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues. New York: Guilford press.

Nordberg, A., Dahlgren Sandberg, A. & Miniscalco, C. (2015). Story retelling and language ability in school-aged children with cerebral palsy and speech impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders / Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists, 50(6), 801-813. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12177

Nordberg, A., Miniscalco, C. & Lohmander, A. (2014). Consonant production and overall speech characteristics in school-aged children with cerebral palsy and speech impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16(4), 386-395. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2014.917440

Nutley, S. B., Darki, F. & Klingberg, T. (2014). Music practice is associated with development of working memory during childhood and adolescence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 926. https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00926

Oakland, T., Douglas, S. & Kane, H. (2016). The top ten standardized tests used with children and youth by school psychologists in 64 countries: A 24 year follow-up study. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 34, 166-176. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282915595303

Olsson, L., Ostergren, R. & Traff, U. (2016). Developmental dyscalculia: A deficit in the approximate number system or an access deficit? Cognitive Development, 39, 154-167. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.04.006

Pind, J., Gunnarsdóttir, E. K. & Jóhannesson, H. S. (2003). Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices: new school age norms and a study of the test's validity. Personality and Individual Differences, 34(3), 375-386. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00058-2

Primi, R. (2001). Complexity of geometric inductive reasoning tasks contribution to the understanding of fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30(1), 41-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00067-8

Raven, J. (1941). Standardization of progressive matrices, 1938. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 19, 137-150. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1941.tb00316.x

Raven, J. (2000a). Psychometrics, cognitive ability, and occupational performance. Review of Psychology, 7(1-2), 51-74.

Raven, J. (2000b). The Raven's Progressive Matrices: Change and Stability over Culture and Time. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 1-48. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0735

Raven, J. (2008a). Manual for the coloured progressive matrices and Crichton vocabulary scale (UK utg.). London: Pearson education.

Raven, J. (2008b). Manual for the standard progressive matrices - plus version and Mill Hill vocabulary scale (UK utg.). London: Pearson education.

Samuelsson, S., Finnstrom, O., Flodmark, O., Gaddlin, P.-O., Leijon, I. & Wadsby, M. (2006). A Longitudinal Study of Reading Skills Among Very-Low-Birthweight Children: Is There a Catch-up? Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31(9), 967-977. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsj108

Schmitt, N. (1996). Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha. Psychological assessment, 8(4), 350-353. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.350

Sijtsma, K. (2009). On the Use, the Misuse, and the Very Limited Usefulness of Cronbach's Alpha. Psychometrika, 74(1), 107-120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-008-9101-0

Skagerlund, K. & Traff, U. (2016a). Number processing and heterogeneity of developmental dyscalculia: Subtypes with different cognitive profiles and deficits. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(1), 36-50. https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219414522707

Skagerlund, K. & Traff, U. (2016b). Processing of space, time, and number contributes to mathematical abilities above and beyond domain-general cognitive abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 143, 85-101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.10.016

Solheim, O. J. (2011). The impact of reading self-efficacy and task value on reading comprehension scores in different item formats. Reading Psychology, 32(1), 1-27. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702710903256601

Svensson, I. & Jacobson, C. (2006). How persistent are phonological difficulties? A longitudinal study of reading retarded children. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 12(1), 3-20. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.296

Söderqvist, S., Nutley, S. B., Ottersen, J., Grill, K. M. & Klingberg, T. (2012). Computerized training of non-verbal reasoning and working memory in children with intellectual disability. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience Vol 6 Oct 2012, ArtID 271. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00271

Thirus, J., Starbrink, M. & Jansson, B. (2016). Relational frame theory, mathematical, and logical skills: A multiple exemplar training intervention to enhance intellectual performance. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 16(2), 141-155.

Träff, U. (2013). The contribution of general cognitive abilities and number abilities to different aspects of mathematics in children (Vol. 116:2, s. 139-156): Journal of experimental child psychology. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.007

Undheim, J. O. (1981). On intelligence I: Broad ability factors in 15-year-old children and Cattell's theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 22, 171–179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.1981.tb00391.x

Undheim, J. O. & Gustafsson, J. E. (1987). The Hierarchical Organization of Cognitive Abilities: Restoring General Intelligence Through the Use of Linear Structural Relations (LISREL). Multivariate Behavioral Research, Vol. 22 (2), 149-171. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2202_2

Vaskinn, A. & Egeland, J. (2012). Testbruksundersøkelsen: En oversikt over tester brukt av norske psykologer. Tidsskrift for norsk psykologforening, 49, 658-665. https://psykologtidsskriftet.no/fagartikkel/2012/07/testbruksundersokelsen-en-oversikt-over-tester-brukt-av-norske-psykologer

Vejleskov, H. (1968). An analysis of Raven matrix responses in fifth grade children: I. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 9(3), 177-186. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.1968.tb00533.x

Wolff, U. & Gustafsson, J.-E. (2015). Structure of phonological ability at age four. Intelligence, 53, 108-117. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.003

Wongupparaj, P., Kumari, V. & Morris, R. G. (2015). A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Raven's Progressive Matrices: Age groups and developing versus developed countries. Intelligence, 49, 1-9. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.008. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.008

Downloads

Additional Files

Published

2017-12-18

How to Cite

Helland-Riise, F., & Martinussen, M. (2017). The Psychometric Properties of the Norwegian version of Ravens Matrices (Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM)/Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM)). PsykTestBarn, 7(2), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.21337/0055

Issue

Section

Kunnskapsoppsummeringer